72 February 2016
human
Kind
New research shows
we are good people,
tolerating bad things.
by George Monbiot
ENVIRONMENT
D
o you find yourself thrashing against the tide
of human indifference and selfishness? Are
you oppressed by the sense that while you
care, others don’t? That because of human-
kind’s callousness, civilisation and the rest
of life on earth are basically stuffed? If so, you are not
alone. But neither are you right.
A study by the Common Cause Foundation, due to be
published next month, reveals two transformative find-
ings. The first is that a large majority of the 1000 people
they surveyed – 74% – identify more strongly with
unselfish values than with selfish values. This means
that they are more interested in helpfulness, honesty,
forgiveness and justice than in money, fame, status and
power. The second is that a similar majority – 78% –
believes others to be more selfish than they really are.
In other words, we have made a terrible mistake about
other people’s minds.
The revelation that humanitys dominant character-
istic is, er, humanity will come as no surprise to those
who have followed recent developments in behavioural
and social sciences. People, these findings suggest,
are basically and inherently nice.
A review article in the journal Frontiers in
Psychology points out that our behaviour towards
unrelated members of our species is “spectacularly
unusual when compared to other animals”. While
chimpanzees might share food with members of their
own group, though usually only after being plagued
by aggressive begging, they tend to react violently
towards strangers. Chimpanzees, the authors note,
behave more like the Homo economicus of neoliberal
mythology than people do.
Humans, by contrast, are ultra-social: possessed of
an enhanced capacity for empathy, an unparalleled
sensitivity to the needs of others, a unique level of con-
cern about their welfare and an ability to create moral
norms that generalise and enforce these tendencies.
Such traits emerge so early in our lives that they
appear to be innate. In other words, it seems that we
have evolved to be this way. By the age of 14 months,
children begin to help each other, for example by hand-
ing over objects another child can’t reach. By the time
they are two, they start sharing things they value. By
the age of three, they start to protest against other peo-
ple’s violation of moral norms.
A fascinating paper in the journal Infancy reveals that
reward has nothing to do with it. Three to five-year-olds
74% Identify more strongly
with unselfish values than
with selsh values
74%
February 2016 73
are less likely to help someone a second time if they
have been rewarded for doing it the first time. In other
words, extrinsic rewards appear to undermine the
intrinsic desire to help. (Parents, economists and gov-
ernment ministers, please note). The study also
discovered that children of this age are more inclined
to help people if they perceive them to be suffering, and
that they want to see someone helped whether or not
they do it themselves. This suggests that they are moti-
vated by a genuine concern for other people’s welfare,
rather than by a desire to look good. And it seems to be
baked in.
Why? How would the hard logic
of evolution produce such out-
comes? This is the subject of
heated debate. One school of
thought contends that altruism is a
logical response to living in small
groups of closely related people,
and evolution has failed to catch up
with the fact that we now live in
large groups, mostly composed of
strangers. Another argues that
large groups containing high
numbers of altruists will outcompete large groups
which contain high numbers of selfish people. A third
hypothesis insists that a tendency towards collabora-
tion enhances your own survival, regardless of the
group in which you might find yourself. Whatever the
mechanism might be, the outcome should be a cause
of celebration.
So why do we retain such a dim view of human
nature? Partly, perhaps, for historical reasons. Philoso-
phers from Hobbes to Rousseau, Malthus to
Schopenhauer, whose understanding of human evolu-
tion was limited to the Book of Genesis, produced
persuasive, influential and catastrophically mistaken
accounts of “the state of nature” (our innate, ancestral
characteristics). Their speculations on this subject
should long ago have been parked on a high shelf
marked “historical curiosities”. But somehow they still
seem to exert a grip on our minds.
Another problem is that – almost by definition – many
of those who dominate public life have a peculiar fixa-
tion on fame, money and power. Their extreme
self-centredness places them in a small minority, but,
because we see them everywhere, we assume that they
are representative of humanity.
The media worships wealth and power, and some-
times launches furious attacks on people who behave
altruistically. In the Daily Mail recently, Richard Little-
john described former Labour leadership candidate
Yvette Cooper’s decision to open her home to refugees
as proof that “noisy emoting has replaced quiet intel-
ligence” (quiet intelligence being one of his defining
qualities). “Its all about political opportunism and
humanitarian posturing,” he theorised, before boast-
ing that he doesn’t “give a damn” about the suffering
of people fleeing Syria. I note with interest the platform
given to people who speak and write as if they are
psychopaths.
The consequences of an undue pessimism about
human nature are momentous. As the Common Cause
Foundation’s survey and interviews reveal, those who
have the bleakest view of humanity are the least likely
to vote. What’s the point, they reason, if everyone else
votes only in their own selfish interests? Interestingly,
and alarmingly for people of my political persuasion, it
also discovered that that liberals tend to possess a
dimmer view of other people than conservatives do. Do
you want to grow the electorate? Do you want progres-
sive politics to flourish? Then spread the word that
other people are broadly well-intentioned.
Misanthropy grants a free pass to the grasping,
power-mad minority who tend to dominate our political
systems. If only we knew how unusual they are, we
might be more inclined to shun them and seek better
leaders. It contributes to the real danger we confront:
not a general selfishness, but a general passivity. Bil-
lions of decent people tut and shake their heads as the
world burns, immobilised by the conviction that no one
else cares.
You are not alone. The world is with you, even if it has
not found its voice.
This article first appeared in the Guardian.
www.monbiot.com
As the Common Cause
Foundations survey and
interviews reveal, those
who have the bleakest
view of humanity are the
least likely to vote
78% believe others to
be more selfish than
they really are
78%

Loading

Back to Top